telescopeѲptics.net
▪
▪
▪
▪
▪▪▪▪
▪
▪
▪
▪
▪
▪
▪
▪
▪ CONTENTS ◄ 12.4. Telescope eyepiece: comparative raytracing ▐ 13.3. Eye aberrations ► 13. THE TELESCOPIC EYE
PAGE HIGHLIGHTS The light passing through the telescope objective and eyepiece is focused onto retina by the optics of the eye, after which it is neurally processed into the visual image. Hence it is the optical media of the eye - cornea, crystalline lens, and aqueous fluid filling the eye - that determine the final shape of the wavefront reaching retinal photoreceptors. Being bio-engineered, eye optics is far from high standards of a quality telescope. However, due to its small aperture and low magnification, significant aberrations generated by it generally do not have much of an effect on the perceived image quality. The two exceptions are eye defocus error - which is effectively corrected by appropriately defocusing the eyepiece, thus inconsequential for the telescope user - and, to a smaller extent, eye astigmatism. Placing eye at the eyepiece end of a telescope changes its optical parameters. This change is significant enough to justify establishing a specific term for this particular mode of operation: the telescopic eye. The most important change is that its aperture stop is now the eyepiece exit pupil, not the iris. This not only directly determines the level of eye aberrations due to its effective aperture, but also may induce additional aberrations resulting from displaced stop. In addition, unlike the "unarmed" eye, which observes objects directly, the telescopic eye observes diffracted image of these objects. In general, the consequences of it are not significant, but should be addressed nevertheless. Neural response and processing of light signals in the eye varies significantly with their intensity and wavelength, making it an important subject for the telescope user. In evaluating the properties of telescopic eye, the starting point is, unavoidably, optical properties of the eye alone. 13.1. The human eye: Physical properties, transmittance and acuityThe entire purpose of a visual telescope is to gather light from distant objects and to enlarge the incoming light angles, so that these objects appear brighter, larger and more detailed to the eye. Added benefit of the larger aperture of a telescope is lessening limitations to image quality imposed by diffraction. Since the light passes through both, telescope system and the eye, optical properties of the latter can affect the final image created by the brain. Being an optical element itself, the eye is, just as the telescope objective, subjected to the effects of diffraction of light and wavefront aberrations. Physical and optical properties of human eye vary individually, often significantly; those presented here are based on experimentally determined averages (FIG. 216).
Retinal arc extends ~32mm through the central meridian. Outer retinal area of relatively low sensitivity to daylight surrounds the yellowish oval spot of ~4mm (nearly 15°) in diameter, centered at ~3.4mm (nearly 12°) from the optical axis of the eye, called macula, which converges toward fovea, the highest daylight sensitivity area. From the outskirts of macula outwards, roughly 20° wide, extends the ring-shaped area of the highest sensitivity to low-intensity light. Eye light transmittance is relatively high in the 500nm-700nm range (and beyond, into infrared), but falling off quickly toward the blue/violet end of the spectrum (FIG. 217).
FIGURE
217: (A) Range of eye spectral transmittance, based on several
small-scale studies. The results indicate wide individual
differences, although it could also result from small sample sizes (four
to nine individuals in four separate studies) and/or differences in
procedures. It is unclear whether eye transmittance - specifically
its preference for mid- and longer wavelengths within the visual
spectrum - has been factored out from the eye spectral response (sensitivity)
curve. If not,
it would superficially lower
actual
sensitivity of the eye in the blue/violet relative to that in the green and red for
both, cones and rods. Since the relative change in transmittance
over the range of wavelengths doesn't seem to vary significantly
with the transmission level, it shouldn't affect individual
perception of chromatism. Variations in eye transmittance would mainly
affect perceived brightness, with the difference between high and low
transmission level being close to one magnitude, roughly evenly across
the visible spectrum (possible exception is that some individuals
may have the ability to sense wavelengths well below 400nm, and some not). Eye photoreceptors cells, cones and rods, form the light-sensing lining of the retina. We need them to sense light, just as we need nerve endings in the skin to sense touch. They range in size from ~2μ to over 10μ, in general becoming larger toward the outer area of the retina, the cones more so than the rods. Dominant retinal photoreceptors at medium-to-small pupil sizes (daylight and indoor light conditions) are the cones, while at large pupil sizes (in low-light conditions) the dominant photoreceptors are the rods. The two differ significantly in, among other properties, their respective resolution limits. Eye resolution level is termed acuity. It varies over the retina, depending on the receptor type and size. It is also a function of the illumination level (FIG. 218).
The area of highest cone acuity coincides with the area of their highest density and smallest individual size - foveola. Area of the highest rode acuity is just outside the macula, in the ring roughly centered at the fovea, some 10° to 15° in radius. Highest acuity level doesn't coincide with the highest image quality, in terms of contrast level. For the naked eye, retinal images are of the highest quality at a pupil diameter of ~2mm (which means in bright-light conditions with the cones dominant), when the combined effect of aberrations and diffraction is at its lowest. In regard to point-image resolution, it is better at ~4mm pupil size (in dim light conditions), with the cones still sufficiently active, diffraction disc is half the size of the disc at 2mm pupil, and the aberration level of ~0.15 wave RMS still doesn't significantly affect the size of central diffraction disc, thus neither resolution of near-equal intensity point sources. However, for most other detail forms, resolution is inferior to that at 2mm pupil size. Retinal resolution shown at left applies to the naked eye, which forms its own point-source diffraction image. For the telescopic eye, there is no point sources, since it images (through the eyepiece) the Airy disc formed by the objective. Hence, given aberration level of the objective, it is the level of eye aberrations that determines image quality which, in general, favors smaller eyepiece exit pupil (this, in turn, favors smaller apertures, with smaller exit pupil for given nominal magnification). However, this effect is, after a certain level, outweighed by the negative effects of higher magnifications. 13.2. EYE AT THE TELESCOPE END At the telescope end, eye plays a dual role; it is an optical element and a photo-detector. Eye optics preceding retinal photoreceptors determines quality of the image formed at the receptors, while size and photo-sensitivity of the receptors, combined with the modes of neural processing of their input, determine perceived quality of his image. Optical part of the eye, consisting of the cornea, eye lens and aqueous fluid, is a simple 2-element system. Expectedly, it generates significant aberrations, on- and off-axis. Eye off-axis aberrations are generally irrelevant, since fixation onto selected object by eye movements brings that object onto the visual axis, with its image falling onto fovea. Image quality in the outer field quickly decreases, but it is inconsequential since the field of high acuity is narrow, and both increasing size and pooled circuitry of the outer retinal receptors farther from central retina - as opposed to individual circuitry of foveal receptors - actually set acuity limit for the outer field, rather than its inherent aberration level. On-axis eye aberration, on the other hand, have disproportionally small effect on perceived image quality. That is mainly the consequence of low effective magnification (i.e. small size of the Airy disc on retina) of the image formed by eye. However, the magnitude of eye aberrations - in particular, defocus and (central) astigmatism - is commonly large enough to noticeably degrade image quality in daily life. Luckily, telescope users are given two important breaks: eye defocus error is effectively corrected by the offsetting error in focusing the eyepiece, and effective eye pupil (determined by the exit pupil of eyepiece) at higher magnifications is small, exponentially lowering eye aberration level. Placed behind telescope eyepiece, eye is looking at the image formed by objective that is magnified by the eyepiece. The corresponding optical scheme is different than for the eye looking at an object directly (FIG. 219).
Since the object image formed by the objective is subjected to its diffraction, every object point in it is replaced by a diffraction pattern formed by the objective. For 0.55μ wavelength, the true angular size of the Airy disc formed by the objective is 4.6/D in arc minutes, for aperture diameter D in mm. The same relation applies to the final image formed in the eye, corrected by the factor of magnification. The corresponding apparent angular Airy disc size is 4.6F/fEP. This is larger than Airy disc of the objective 4.6/D by a factor of f/f EP - equaling telescope magnification - where f is the telescope focal length.
As long as the exit pupil is smaller than eye pupil,
there is no additional diffraction effect for the image formed on the retina.
It is because the exit pupil wavefront, as small as it is, is
still a part of the continuous optical train, starting with the telescope aperture
stop (just like the Gregorian secondary, placed after the intermediate focus,
doesn't induce additional diffraction, only magnifies). If, however, exit pupil
becomes larger than eye pupil, the physical diffraction pattern on the retina becomes larger
correspondingly to the change in f-ratio, which with the eye focal length unchanged,
results in as much larger angular size. While it is still the apparent size, with the
image scale unchanged it has the effect of that much larger true angular Airy disc.
It also results in a light loss, proportional to (D'/D)2 for unobstructed, and
[(D'-o)/(D-o)]2 for obstructed apertures (D, D' and o being
the original aperture, the effective aperture, and the central obstruction, all normalized for D=1).
The relative obstruction size for the latter also increases, but the effect is negligible due to very low magnifications.
Since exit pupil formed by the eyepiece is a real image of the objective,
it is as such present in the eye pupil area. Duality of the exit pupil as
being "made" of two different wave sources coming from the objective
is hinted at on FIG.209: waves from each
point of the objective focusing into image points merely pass through
the exit pupil as collimated pencils after being refracted by the eyepiece, while each point
of the objective fills the field stop with light that actually focuses onto
the exit pupil and forms real
image of the objective (for the central point this light is outlined above axis within
the angle α). With the image field
seen through the eyepiece projected at infinity, this real image is suspended
in the view, but usually not detectable. Physiologically, image field
is produced from beams focused through the exit pupil onto retinal receptors, but there is
also light fallout on them from the exit pupil as an object on its own.
Image below illustrates location of exit pupil and its dual nature.
Pupil is formed as a final image in front of the eye pupil,
after refraction through cornea (note that in the presence of obstruction
each pencil has a dark core, but it is not detectable in the point
image on the retina, no matter how big the relative obstruction).
There is no focused light from the exit pupil on the retina, so there is
no real, nor virtual image on it (thus somewhat smaller than the
axial pencil exiting the eyepiece). Parallel pencils of light pass through
it, but any random point A on it, formed by the light originating on the
corresponding point in the aperture, emits diverging cone of light
toward the eye lens. It is way too close to be imaged on the retina,
but light close to the chief - i.e. middle - ray, being nearly
parallel, does get processed
by the lens, bent toward retina and at least partly focused on it.
This is a small fraction of
the total light emited by the point, making this ghost image
very low in intensity. However, in the presence of
central obstruction, it can become visible if the background is
relatively bright (as if in daylight observing, or observing bright objects).
This intermediate image does contribute
light energy to the receptors, projecting onto the objective's image.
The bright portion of it makes it brigher,
and the annulus area - when present - not, or in effect makes it darker; that can create a
ghost image of the annulus suspended in front of the real image of the observed object.
Larger relative size of the obstruction in eye pupil
(e.g. with exit pupil larger than eye pupil) make the obstruction shadow appear
larger, but the main factor is pupil brightness (which is also
dependant on magnification).
◄ 12.4. Telescope eyepiece: comparative raytracing ▐ 13.3. Eye aberrations ►
|